The influence of analogies on the development of selective attention in novices in normal or stressful conditions

  1. Afroditi Lola 1
  2. George Giatsis 1
  3. José Antonio Pérez Turpin 2
  4. George Tzetzis 1
  1. 1 Aristotle University of Thessaloniki
    info

    Aristotle University of Thessaloniki

    Salónica, Grecia

    ROR https://ror.org/02j61yw88

  2. 2 Universitat d'Alacant
    info

    Universitat d'Alacant

    Alicante, España

    ROR https://ror.org/05t8bcz72

Revista:
Journal of Human Sport and Exercise: JHSE

ISSN: 1988-5202

Año de publicación: 2023

Volumen: 18

Número: 1

Páginas: 139-152

Tipo: Artículo

DOI: 10.14198/JHSE.2023.181.12 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openRUA editor

Otras publicaciones en: Journal of Human Sport and Exercise: JHSE

Objetivos de desarrollo sostenible

Resumen

The effect of explicit, implicit, and analogy learning methods on developing selective attention skills in young novice participants was examined under normal and stressful conditions. The 60 novice participants, aged 11-12 years old, followed a video simulation intervention program of 12 sessions (3 times * 4 weeks). They were tested four times (pre-test, post-test, retention, and stress test) to evaluate their reaction time and accuracy in selective attention skills. The differences among the groups and the measurements were analysed using a two-way mixed factorial ANOVA (4 groups * 4 measurements), and post-hoc Bonferroni evaluated significant differences among the groups. The results showed that all three experimental groups improved their performance in response reaction time and accuracy from the pre-test to the post-test and maintained their scores on the retention test. However, the analogy group had the best scores, followed by the implicit group, and they were both better than the explicit group. According to the stress test, the analogy and the implicit group remained unaffected while the explicit group showed a decrease in both reaction time and accuracy. Training by analogy was more effective in improving selective attention in novices in both normal and stressful conditions. Analogy learners probably developed an advanced mechanism that allowed them to quickly and accurately select the most useful information from the sport setting even in stressful conditions. This research highlights the advantages of simulation techniques; future research may assess selective attention in real conditions.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci, 4(10), 829-839. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201
  • Behan, M., & Wilson, M. (2008). State anxiety and visual attention: The role of the quiet eye period in aiming to a far target. J Sport Sci, 26(2), 207-215. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640410701446919
  • Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure?. J Exp Psychol: General, 130(4), 701. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.701
  • Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2004). 14. From novice to expert performance. In: A. M. Williams & N. J. Hodges (Eds.), Skill acquisition in sport: Research, theory and practice (pp. 309-328). Routledge.
  • Bennett, S. J. (2000). Implicit learning: Should it be used in practice?. In Scientific Sport Psychology Workshop, Nov. 1997, Amsterdam, Netherlands. Edizioni Luigi Pozzi.
  • Buszard, T., Farrow, D., Zhu, F. F., & Masters, R. S. (2016). The relationship between working memory capacity and cortical activity during performance of a novel motor task. Psychol Sport Exerc, 22, 247-254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.07.005
  • Davids, K., Williams, A.M., Button, C, and Court, M (2001). An integrative modeling approach to the study of intentional movement behavior. In: R. N. Singer, H. A. Hausenblas, & C. Janelle, (Eds.), Handbook of Sport Psychology (pp. 144-173). 2nd edition. John Wiley & Sons.
  • Englert, C., & Oudejans, R. R. (2014). Is choking under pressure a consequence of skill-focus or increased distractibility? Results from a tennis serve task. Psychology, 5(9), 1035-1043. https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2014.59116
  • Ewolds, H. E., Bröker, L., De Oliveira, R. F., Raab, M., & Künzell, S. (2017). Implicit and explicit knowledge both improve dual task performance in a continuous pursuit tracking task. Front Psychol, 8, 2241. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02241
  • Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognit Emot, 6(6), 409-434. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939208409696
  • Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336
  • Gentner, D., & Forbus, K. D. (2011). Computational models of analogy. Wires Cogn Sci, 2(3), 266-276. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.105
  • Gotardi, G. C., Polastri, P. F., Schor, P., Oudejans, R. R., van der Kamp, J., Savelsbergh, G. J., ... & Rodrigues, S. T. (2019). Adverse effects of anxiety on attentional control differ as a function of experience: A simulated driving study. Applied Ergonomics, 74, 41-47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2018.08.009
  • Gröpel, P., & Mesagno, C. (2019). Choking interventions in sports: A systematic review. Int Rev Sport Exer P, 12(1), 176-201. https://doi.org/10.1080/1750984X.2017.1408134
  • Hayes, S., Hirsch, C., & Mathews, A. (2008). Restriction of working memory capacity during worry. J Abnorm Psychol, 117(3), 712. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012908
  • Holroyd, C. B., Yeung, N., Coles, M. G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). A mechanism for error detection in speeded response time tasks. J Exp Psychol: General, 134(2), 163. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.163
  • Jackson, R. C., & Farrow, D. (2005). Implicit perceptual training: How, when, and why?. Hum Movement Sci, 24(3), 308-325. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2005.06.003
  • Janelle, C. M., & Hillman, C. H. (2003). Expert performance in sport. Current perspectives and critical issues. In J. L. Starkes & K.A. Ericsson (Eds.), Expert Performance In Sports: Advances in Research on Sport Expertise (pp. 19-47). Human Kinetics.
  • Kleynen, M., Braun, S. M., Bleijlevens, M. H., Lexis, M. A., Rasquin, S. M., Halfens, J., ... & Masters, R. S. (2014). Using a Delphi technique to seek consensus regarding definitions, descriptions and classification of terms related to implicit and explicit forms of motor learning. PLoS One, 9(6), e100227. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100227
  • Knudsen, E. I. (2007). Fundamental components of attention. Annu Rev Neurosci, 30, 57-78. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094256
  • Koedijker, J. M., Poolton, J. M., Maxwell, J. P., Oudejans, R. R., Beek, P. J., & Masters, R. S. (2011). Attention and time constraints in perceptual-motor learning and performance: Instruction, analogy, and skill level. Conscious Cogn, 20(2), 245-256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.08.002
  • Komar, J., Chow, J. Y., Chollet, D., & Seifert, L. (2014). Effect of analogy instructions with an internal focus on learning a complex motor skill. J Appl Sport Psychol, 26(1), 17-32. https://doi.org/10.1080/10413200.2013.771386
  • Lam, W. K., Maxwell, J. P., & Masters, R. (2009). Analogy learning and the performance of motor skills under pressure. J Sport Exercise Psy, 31(3), 337-357. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.31.3.337
  • Lam, W. K., Maxwell, J. P., & Masters, R. S. W. (2009). Analogy versus explicit learning of a modified basketball shooting task: Performance and kinematic outcomes. J Sport Sci, 27(2), 179-191. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640410802448764
  • Lam, W. K., Maxwell, J. P., & Masters, R. S. W. (2010). Probing the allocation of attention in implicit (motor) learning. J Sport Sci, 28(14), 1543-1554. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2010.517543
  • Law, J., Masters, R., Bray, S. R., Eves, F., & Bardswell, I. (2003). Motor performance as a function of audience affability and metaknowledge. J Sport Exercise Psy, 25(4), 484-500. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.25.4.484
  • Lee, T.D., Chamberlin, C.J., Hodges, N.J. (2001). Practice. In: R. N. Singer, H. A. Hausenblas, & C. Jannelle (Eds.), Handbook of Sport Psychology (pp. 115-143). 2nd edition. John Wiley & Sons.
  • Lewis, B. P., & Linder, D. E. (1997). Thinking about choking? Attentional processes and paradoxical performance. Per Soc Psychol B, 23(9), 937-944. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167297239003
  • Liao, C. M., & Masters, R. S. (2001). Analogy learning: A means to implicit motor learning. J Sport Sci, 19(5), 307-319. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640410152006081
  • Lohse, K. R., Wulf, G., & Ite, R. L. W. (2012). Attentional focus affects movement efficiency. In Skill Acquisition in Sport (pp. 66-84). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203133712-12
  • Lola, A., Koutsomarkou, A., & Tzetzis, G. (2021). Effective methods of developing simple or complex perceptual-motor skills in children. Interciencia Journal, 46(3), in press.
  • Lola, A., Koutsomarkou, A., & Tzetzis, G. (2021). Influence of different focus of attention instructions on learning volleyball skills for young novices. Journal of Human Sport and Exercise, 17(4), in press. https://doi.org/10.14198/jhse.2022.174.14
  • Lola A., & Tzetzis, G. (2020). Analogy versus explicit and implicit learning of a volleyball skill for novices: The effect on motor performance and self-efficacy. Journal of Physical Education and Sport, 20, 2478 - 2486.
  • Lola A., Tzetzis G., & Zetou E. (2012). The effect of implicit and explicit practice in the development of decision making in volleyball serving. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 114(2), 1-14. https://doi.org/10.2466/05.23.25.PMS.114.2.665-678
  • Lola, A. C., & Tzetzis, G. C. (2021). The effect of explicit, implicit and analogy instruction on decision making skill for novices, under stress. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1-21. https://doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2021.1877325
  • MacMahon, K., & Masters, R. S. (2002). The effects of secondary tasks on implicit motor skill performance. Int J Sport Psychol, 33, 307-324.
  • Martens, R., Vealey, R. and Burton, D. (1990). Competitive Anxiety in Sport. Human Kinetics.
  • Masters, R. S. (1992). Knowledge, knerves and know‐how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. Brit J Psychol, 83(3), 343-358. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1992.tb02446.x
  • Masters, R. S. (2000). Theoretical aspects of implicit learning in sport. Int J Sport Psychol, 31(4), 530-541.
  • Maxwell, J. P., Masters, R. S. W., & Eves, F. F. (2003). The role of working memory in motor learning and performance. Conscious Cogn, 12(3), 376-402. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00005-9
  • Maxwell, J. P., Masters, R. S., & Eves, F. F. (2000). From novice to no know-how: A longitudinal study of implicit motor learning. J Sport Sci, 18(2), 111-120. https://doi.org/10.1080/026404100365180
  • Memmert, D. (2009). Pay attention! A review of visual attentional expertise in sport. Int Rev Sport Exer P, 2(2), 119-138. https://doi.org/10.1080/17509840802641372
  • Neumann, O. (1984). 17. Automatic processing: A review of recent findings and a plea for an old theory. In: W. Prinz & A.F. Sanders (Eds.), Cognition and Motor Processes, 255-293. Springer-Verlag. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-69382-3_17
  • Ong, N. T. T., Bowcock, A., & Hodges, N. J. (2010). Manipulations to the timing and type of instructions to examine motor skill performance under pressure. Front Psychol, 1, 196. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00196
  • Poolton, J. M., Masters, R. S. W., & Maxwell, J. P. (2005). The relationship between initial errorless learning conditions and subsequent performance. Hum Movement Sci, 24(3), 362-378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2005.06.006
  • Poolton, J. M., Masters, R. S., & Maxwell, J. P. (2006). The influence of analogy learning on decision-making in table tennis: Evidence from behavioural data. Psychol Sport Exerc 7(6), 677-688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.03.005
  • Poolton, J. M., Masters, R. S., & Maxwell, J. P. (2007). Passing thoughts on the evolutionary stability of implicit motor behaviour: Performance retention under physiological fatigue. Conscious Cogn, 16(2), 456-468. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2006.06.008
  • Poolton, J. M., Maxwell, J. P., Masters, R. S. W., & Raab, M. (2006). Benefits of an external focus of attention: Common coding or conscious processing?. J Sport Sci, 24(1), 89-99. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640410500130854
  • Schmader, T., & Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. J Pers Soc Psychol, 85(3), 440. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.440
  • Smeeton, N. J., Williams, A. M., Hodges, N. J., & Ward, P. (2005). The relative effectiveness of various instructional approaches in developing anticipation skill. J Exp Psychol-Appl, 11(2), 98. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.11.2.98
  • Tsorbatzoudis, H., Barkoukis, V., Kaissidis-Rodafinos, A., & Grouios, G. (1998). A test of the reliability and factorial validity of the Greek version of the CSAI-2. Res Q Exercise Sport, 69(4), 416-419. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.1998.10607717
  • Tzetzis G., & Lola A. (2010). The role of implicit, explicit instruction, and their combination in learning anticipation skill, under normal and stress conditions. International Journal of Sport Sciences and Physical Education, 1, 54-59.
  • Uehara, L. A., Button, C., & Davids, K. (2008). The effects of focus of attention instructions on novices learning soccer chip. Braz J Biomotricity, 2(1), 63-77.
  • van der Graaff, E., Hoozemans, M., Pasteuning, M., Veeger, D., & Beek, P. J. (2018). Focus of attention instructions during baseball pitching training. Int J Sport Sci Coach, 13(3), 391-397. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747954117711095
  • Van der Kamp, J., Oudejans, R., & Savelsbergh, G. (2003). The development and learning of the visual control of movement: An ecological perspective. Infant Behav Dev, 26(4), 495-515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.09.002
  • Vickers, J. N., & Williams, A. M. (2007). Performing under pressure: The effects of physiological arousal, cognitive anxiety, and gaze control in biathlon. J Motor Behav, 39(5), 381-394. https://doi.org/10.3200/JMBR.39.5.381-394
  • Votsis, E., Tzetzis, G., Hatzitaki, V., & Grouios, V. G. (2009). The effect of implicit and explicit methods in acquisition of anticipation skill in low and high complexity situations. Int J Sport Psychol, 40(3), 374-391.
  • Williams, A. M., Ward, P., & Chapman, C. (2003). Training perceptual skill in field hockey: Is there transfer from the laboratory to the field?. Res Q Exercise Sport, 74(1), 98-103. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2003.10609068
  • Wilson, M. R., Vine, S. J., & Wood, G. (2009). The influence of anxiety on visual attentional control in basketball free throw shooting. J Sport Exercise Psy, 31(2), 152-168. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.31.2.152
  • Wood, G., & Wilson, M. R. (2010). A moving goalkeeper distracts penalty takers and impairs shooting accuracy. J Sport Sci, 28(9), 937-946. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2010.495995
  • Wulf, G. (2007). Attention and motor skill learning. Human Kinetics. https://doi.org/10.5040/9781492596844
  • Wulf, G. (2013). Attentional focus and motor learning: a review of 15 years. Int Rev Sport Exer P, 6(1), 77-104. https://doi.org/10.1080/1750984X.2012.723728
  • Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2010). Effortless motor learning? An external focus of attention enhances movement effectiveness and efficiency. In: B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless attention: A new perspective in attention and action (pp. 75-101). MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262013840.003.0004